True story: Narrative as an adaptation for the communication of social survival information

Rayn Epremian, Jonas Nölle, Christine Cuskley and Matthew Spike

Storytelling is nearly as universal and central to human cognition as language. Narrative has been identified as the way humans process experiences (Wilkens et al, 2005), store memories (Nelson, 1993; Schank & Abelson, 1995), and form personal identities (Young & Saver, 2001; Yuval-Davis, 2006). Gottschall (2012, p. 65) proposed that mental rehearsal through story prepares us to react to similar events in the future. Zipes (2006, p. 11) argues that stories survive transmission over generations when they contain information that helps to preserve a community, and this information is largely social (Zipes, 2006, p. 22; Gottschall, 2012, p. 67). Social information survives cultural transmission because it is a kind of survival information, helping individuals learn to thrive in a social context, thereby bolstering survival and fitness.

Previous studies have used iterated learning to examine the role of cultural transmission in the evolution of language (Smith, Kirby & Brighton, 2003; Kirby, Cornish & Smith, 2008; Kirby, Griffiths & Smith, 2014), and other transmission chain studies have revealed content biases for social and survival information in the transmission of narratives (Bartlett, 1920; Stubbersfield, Tehrani & Flynn, 2014; 2017). This experiment combines the transmission chain paradigm with task-based experiments (Bahrami et al, 2010; Bjørndahl et al, 2014; Thompson et al, 1993) to test whether exposure to social survival information in the form of narrative will improve performance in a social task, providing experimental evidence that stories function as ‘problem simulation[s]’ (Gottschall, 2012 p. 63). Participants undertook a social task: a role-playing game in which they had to learn a set of novel, arbitrary social customs to succeed. In the first generation, participants learned by choosing behaviours during play without any prior input, but received feedback on their behaviour throughout the game. In two conditions, they then wrote either a story or list, designed to memorably communicate the social customs to thenext player (see demo here: https://bit.ly/narrative-game). Subsequent generations of players then received the story or list of the previous player prior to play, which was otherwise identicalto the first generation. Participants were scored on their performance during play based on whether they chose the “correct” behaviours. Scores in the social task improved over generations, supporting the hypothesis that narratives effectively communicate social survival information, with score increasing by a mean value of 1.02 points (SE =±0.24, t=4.2) per generation across conditions. Qualitative analysis showed that the narratives produced by participants were highly creative, exhibiting a range of communication strategies and narrative elements (e.g.,. causality, setting, and agency).

Taken together, our findings support the hypothesis that narratives effectively communicate social survival information in transmission, a result which is compatible with Dunbar’s (1997) social brain hypothesis, the ‘problem simulation theory of storytelling’ (Gottschall, 2012 p. 63), and previous experimental work on content biases (Stubbersfield, Tehrani & Flynn 2014, 2017). We provide a novel experimental paradigm which can be adapted to target more specific questions about the evolution of narratives.

References

Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Latham, P. E., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Optimally Interacting Minds. Science, 329(5995), 1081–1085. doi: 10.1126/science.1185718

Bartlett, F. C. (1920). Some Experiments on the Reproduction of Folk-Stories. Folklore, 31(1), 30–47. doi: 10.1080/0015587x.1920.9719123

Bjørndahl, J. S., Fusaroli, R., Østergaard, S., & Tylén, K. (2014). Thinking together with material representations: Joint epistemic actions in creative problem solving. Cognitive Semiotics, 7(1), 103–123. doi: 10.1515/cogsem-2014-0006

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1997). Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber and Faber.

Gottschall, J. (2012). The storytelling animal: How stories make us human. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

Kirby, S., Griffiths, T., & Smith, K. (2014). Iterated learning and the evolution of language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 108-114. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2014.07.014

Nelson, K. (1993). The Psychological and Social Origins of Autobiographical Memory. Psychological Science, 4(1), 7–14. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00548.x

Schank, R. C. & Abelson, R. P. (1995) Knowledge and Memory: The Real Story. In: Robert S. Wyer, Jr (ed) Knowledge and Memory: The Real Story. Hillsdale, NJ. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 1-85.

Stubbersfield, J., & Tehrani, J. (2012). Expect the Unexpected? Testing for Minimally Counterintuitive (MCI) Bias in the Transmission of Contemporary Legends. Social Science Computer Review, 31(1), 90–102. doi: 10.1177/0894439312453567

Stubbersfield, J. M., Tehrani, J. J., & Flynn, E. G. (2014). Serial killers, spiders and cybersex: Social and survival information bias in the transmission of urban legends. British Journalof Psychology, 106(2), 288–307. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12073

Stubbersfield, J. M., Flynn, E. G., & Tehrani, J. J. (2017). Cognitive Evolution and the Transmission of Popular Narratives: A Literature Review and Application to Urban Legends. Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture, 1(1), 121–136. doi:10.26613/esic/1.1.120

Wilkens, T., Hughes, A., Wildemuth, B. M., & Marchionini, G. (2005). The role of narrative inunderstanding digital video: An exploratory analysis. Proceedings of the American Society forInformation Science and Technology, 40(1), 323–329. doi:10.1002/meet.1450400140

Young, K., & Saver, J. (2001). The Neurology of Narrative. SubStance, 30(1/2), 72-84. doi:10.2307/3685505

Yuval-Davis, N. (2006) Belonging and the politics of belonging, Patterns of Prejudice, 40:3,197-214, DOI:10.1080/00313220600769331

Zipes, J. (2006). Why fairy tales stick: The evolution and relevance of a genre. New York: Routledge.